Journal Article Reviews

Psychological Science
21(12) 1863 –1870
© The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797610389186
http://pss.sagepub.com

With the prevalence of overweight and obesity constantly on
the rise, dieting has become the most important strategy to
control weight in food-rich environments (Kruger, Galuska,
Serdula, & Jones, 2004). Although chronic dieters, also
referred to as restrained eaters (Herman & Polivy, 1980), are
generally highly motivated to restrict food intake in order to
control body weight, only a few succeed in losing weight and
in maintaining weight losses over significant periods of time
(Mann et al., 2007; Stroebe, 2008). Precisely why dieters often
fail to meet their dieting goals, however, has been the subject
of continuous debate.

The Hedonic-Response Hypothesis
One recent approach proposes that eating is often driven by the
automatic hedonic processing of food cues, rather than by the
homeostatic regulation of hunger per se (Lowe & Butryn, 2007;
Pinel, Assanand, & Lehman, 2000). Specifically, dieters may
experience stronger hedonic responses to highly palatable-food
cues than normal eaters do. These hedonic responses are
assumed to be responsible for attentional biases to tempting-
food cues (Papies, Stroebe, & Aarts, 2008), the emergence of
food cravings and intrusive thoughts (Kavanagh, Andrade, &
May, 2005; Papies, Stroebe, & Aarts, 2007), and the temporary

inhibition of the long-term dieting goal (for a model, see
Stroebe, Mensink, Aarts, Schut, & Kruglanski, 2008).

Central to this line of reasoning is the assumption that diet-
ers show more positive hedonic responses to tempting-food
cues than normal eaters do. However, previous research using
explicit measures has often failed to find a difference between
dieters and normal eaters in the self-reported evaluation of
palatable-food stimuli (Fedoroff, Polivy, & Herman, 1997;
Stroebe et al., 2008); some such research has even found a
devaluation of highly palatable food among dieters (e.g.,
Papies et al., 2008). It has been argued that hedonic reactions,
because of their spontaneous, automatic nature, may be better
captured with the help of indirect measures (e.g., Hofmann,
Friese, & Strack, 2009). However, consistent with the self-
report data just mentioned, research using indirect measures
has yielded only inconclusive evidence for the hedonic-
response hypothesis, with two studies reporting more positive
hedonic reactions among dieters (Hoefling & Strack, 2008;
Veenstra & de Jong, in press), one study finding no difference

Corresponding Author:
Wilhelm Hofmann, Booth School of , University of Chicago, 5807
South Woodlawn Ave., Chicago, IL 60637
E-mail: [email protected]

As Pleasure Unfolds: Hedonic Responses
to Tempting Food

Wilhelm Hofmann1, Guido M. van Koningsbruggen2,
Wolfgang Stroebe2, Suresh Ramanathan1, and Henk Aarts2
1University of Chicago and 2Utrecht University

Abstract

Why do chronic dieters often violate their dieting goals? One possibility is that they experience stronger hedonic responses
to tempting food than normal eaters do. We scrutinized hedonic processing in dieters and normal eaters (a) by manipulating
food preexposure and (b) by assessing both immediate and delayed hedonic responses to tempting food with an adapted affect-
misattribution procedure. Without food preexposure, dieters showed less positive hedonic responses than normal eaters
(Study 1). When preexposed to tempting-food stimuli, however, dieters exhibited more positive delayed hedonic responses than
normal eaters (Studies 1 and 2). Furthermore, delayed hedonic responding was meaningfully related to self-reported power of
food and state cravings (Study 2). These findings suggest that dieters experience difficulties in down-regulating hedonic affect
when in a “hot” state and that self-regulation research may benefit from a greater emphasis on temporal dynamics rather than
static differences.

Keywords

self-regulation, hedonic processes, affect regulation, eating, dietary restraint

Received 8/17/09; Revision accepted 6/23/10

Research Article

1864 Hofmann et al.

between dieters and normal eaters (Roefs, Herman, Macleod,
Smulders, & Jansen, 2005), and two studies finding more neg-
ative hedonic reactions to palatable food among dieters
(Papies, Stroebe, & Aarts, 2009, Experiment 1; E. Papies, per-
sonal communication, May 4, 2010; Roefs & Jansen, 2002).
How should these seemingly divergent hedonic-response find-
ings be interpreted?

A Two-Factor Approach to Hedonic
Processing
In this article, we argue that the assumption that has guided
previous research—the assumption of a static difference
between dieters and normal eaters—is too simple. We propose
that to understand hedonic responses in eating behavior, and
probably other domains of self-regulation as well, it is neces-
sary to take into account the dynamic interplay of two crucial
factors: preexposure and the time course of hedonic responses.
Preexposure concerns the (often disruptive) influence of the
immediate stimulus environment on self-regulatory processes
(e.g., Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999). In the eating domain, for
example, it has been found that dieters are more likely to over-
eat after they have been preexposed to the sight, smell, or taste
of tempting-food cues (Fedoroff et al., 1997; Fedoroff, Polivy,
& Herman, 2003; Herman & Mack, 1975; Jansen & van den
Hout, 1991; Rogers & Hill, 1989). Research examining the
potential causes of this overeating effect shows that preexpo-
sure to words associated with tempting foods increases atten-
tional bias for tempting food (Papies et al., 2008) and
temporarily inhibits access to the dieting goal (Stroebe et al.,
2008). These findings suggest that dieters may show different
hedonic responses depending on whether or not they have
been brought into a “hot state” by the presence of tempting-
food primes.1

The second crucial factor is the time course of the hedonic
response. Previous research using indirect measures has
exclusively focused on immediate hedonic reactions to stimu-
lus exposure. By taking into account only the immediate
hedonic response, this approach leaves out an important piece
of information—namely, how the mental apparatus deals with
hedonic affect over time. Recent neuroscientific research sug-
gests that evaluation is a fast and dynamic process that
includes multiple iterative processing cycles even within the
first second after stimulus exposure (Cunningham, Zelazo,
Packer, & Van Bavel, 2007). Iterative hedonic processing
may lead to maintenance or even amplification of hedonic
responses if attention is continuously allocated to the affec-
tive information and no regulatory process is recruited
(Kavanagh et al., 2005; Van Dillen & Koole, 2007). Con-
versely, hedonic responses may be down-regulated over time
through reallocation of attention and through mechanisms of
affect regulation that allow the psychological system to disen-
gage from a tempting episode (Hofmann, Friese, & Roefs,
2009).

Study 1

To test the proposed two-factor model, we assessed dieters’
and normal eaters’ immediate as well as delayed hedonic
responses to tempting-food stimuli as a function of preexpo-
sure. We hypothesized that without preexposure, dieters’ diet-
ing goal would be dominant (Stroebe et al., 2008) and would
successfully bias information processing, including the deval-
uation of goal-incompatible temptations (Aarts, Custers, &
Holland, 2007; Myrseth, Fishbach, & Trope, 2009; Veling &
Aarts, 2009). However, we also expected that after preexpo-
sure to tempting food, dieters would become increasingly sen-
sitive to the hedonic aspects of that food; consequently, we
predicted they would (a) exhibit difficulties in down-regulat-
ing positive hedonic affect and (b) show prolonged hedonic
processing over time compared with normal eaters. Further-
more, we assumed that this pattern of effects would be specific
to food stimuli and would not generalize to food-unrelated
control stimuli.

Method
Participants were 80 students from Utrecht University (34 male,
46 female; mean age = 21.75 years, SD = 2.33), who received
either course credit or monetary compensation (approximately
$5). They were assigned randomly to the preexposure or the no-
preexposure condition. Afterward, we unobtrusively measured
immediate and delayed hedonic responses to tempting food
with a modified version of the affect-misattribution procedure
(AMP; Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005). Finally, par-
ticipants filled out a dietary restraint scale.

Food preexposure manipulation. A lexical decision task
was used to implement food preexposure experimentally, as in
Papies et al. (2008). In each trial of the task, participants were
presented with a word or nonword target stimulus, and they
had to indicate quickly via key press whether the presented
stimulus was a word or a nonword. Participants in the preex-
posure condition (n = 39) were presented with 15 words refer-
ring to palatable foods (e.g., pizza, chocolate, cake), 15 neutral
words (e.g., book), and 30 nonwords. Participants in the no-
preexposure condition (n = 41) were presented with 30 neutral
words and 30 nonwords. Presentation order of stimuli was ran-
domized across the 60 trials of the task.

Hedonic response over time. Building on the AMP (Payne
et al., 2005), we developed a new procedure to capture imme-
diate versus delayed hedonic responses to tempting-food stim-
uli (see Hofmann, Friese, & Roefs, 2009, for a first application
assessing general affect regulatory capacity). In a standard
trial of the task, a prime stimulus is presented for 100 ms, fol-
lowed by an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 100 ms (a blank
screen), the presentation of a Chinese pictograph for 200 ms,
and finally a mask, which remains until the participant judges

Hedonic Responses to Tempting Food 1865

the pleasantness of the pictograph (see Fig. S1 in the Supple-
mental Material available online). Standard trials employing
such a short ISI are typically used to measure immediate affec-
tive reactions to the primes (misattributed to the pictographs),
with a higher proportion of “pleasant” judgments indicating a
more positive hedonic response (Payne et al., 2005). We added
a second trial type in which the ISI was increased to 1,000 ms,
keeping all other aspects of the task constant (see Fig. S1).
Comparing responses to identical stimuli at the short versus
the long ISI informed us as to how immediately activated
affect unfolded over time. Specifically, a decrease in positive
responses from the short to the long ISI would be indicative of
a down-regulation of immediate affect over time.

The task consisted of 90 trials, presented in random order.
The primes were 15 pictures of palatable food selected from
a pilot test (N = 24) and 15 positive and 15 negative food-
irrelevant stimuli from the International Affective Picture Sys-
tem (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2005). To allow for a specificity
analysis (see Results and Discussion), we matched the food
and positive control stimuli according to ratings (on 9-point
scales) of valence (M

food
= 6.31, SD = 0.32; M

positive
= 6.32, SD =

0.31) and arousal (M
food

= 5.41, SD = 0.40; M
positive

= 5.31,
SD = 0.39). Each stimulus was shown once in each ISI condi-
tion and was paired with a new pictograph each time. As in
Payne, Burkley, and Stokes’s (2008) study, participants
responded to the pictographs with the index and middle fin-
gers of both hands placed on four keys, labeled “−2” (very
unpleasant), “−1” (slightly unpleasant), “+1” (slightly pleas-
ant), and “+2” (very pleasant). For data analysis, we excluded
responses made less than 150 ms or more than 3,500 ms after
the onset of the pictograph.

Restraint scale. Dietary restraint was assessed with the Con-
cern for Dieting subscale of the Revised Restraint Scale (Her-
man & Polivy, 1980). This subscale consists of six items, and
the score is the sum of ratings on a scale from 0 to 3. The mean
score was 6.60 (SD = 3.40; α = .74).

Results and discussion
To simplify interpretation, we recoded AMP responses to a
scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more positive
responses. We then performed a general linear model (GLM)
analysis on hedonic responses with stimulus type (food vs.
positive control stimuli) and ISI (100 ms vs. 1,000 ms) as
within-subjects factors, food preexposure (present vs. absent)
as a between-subjects factor, and dietary restraint as a continu-
ous predictor (with means for dieters and normal eaters esti-
mated at +1 SD and −1 SD, respectively).2 This analysis
revealed two significant interactions: a three-way interaction
of ISI, food preexposure, and dietary restraint, F(1, 76) = 4.14,
p = .045, η

p
2 = .05, which was qualified by a four-way interac-

tion of stimulus type, ISI, food preexposure, and dietary
restraint, F(1, 76) = 5.51, p = .022, η

p
2 = .07. To decompose

these two interactions, we investigated the effects of ISI and

restraint separately for food and positive control primes and
separately for the no-preexposure and the preexposure condi-
tions within each of the prime conditions.

The analysis for food stimuli revealed that in the
no-preexposure condition, there was only a main effect of
restraint, F(1, 39) = 7.88, p = .008, η

p
2 = .17. As Figure 1

shows, dieters had less positive hedonic reactions to palatable
food than normal eaters did when not preexposed. In the pre-
exposure condition, however, there was a significant interac-
tion between ISI and restraint, F(1, 37) = 6.56, p = .015, η

p
2 =

.15, and no main effects. Hedonic responses of dieters (+1 SD)
and normal eaters (−1 SD) did not differ at the short ISI,
t(37) = 0.42, p = .673, η

p
2 = .005. However, dieters’ hedonic

responses were significantly larger than those of normal eaters
at the long ISI, t(37) = 2.15, p = .038, η

p
2 = .11 (see Fig. 1).

This dissociation in the delayed response was due to a signifi-
cant decline in hedonic responding for normal eaters over
time, t(37) = –2.79, p = .008, η

p
2 = .17. In contrast, dieters

maintained high levels of positive responding over time, t(37) =
0.74, p = .46, η

p
2 = .02. The analysis for positive control stim-

uli yielded neither significant main effects of ISI and restraint
nor a significant interaction between ISI and restraint in either
condition, all Fs < 1.05 (see Fig. 1). Thus, the difference between dieters and normal eaters was specific to food stimuli. The results from Study 1 suggest that without preexposure to food, dieters devalue palatable-food primes effectively even at very short time frames. Thus, implicit self-control appears to be the default for dieters (Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglan- ski, 2003). When preexposed to palatable food, however, diet- ers showed elevated hedonic responses that persisted over time, whereas normal eaters showed signs of down-regulation over time. Thus, dieters’ exposure to tempting food triggers a hot state during which they become increasingly sensitive— and vulnerable—to the hedonic aspects of food. Study 2 The aim of Study 2 was to examine more closely the time pat- tern that emerged with food preexposure in Study 1. Specifi- cally, we wanted to investigate whether the observed difference between dieters’ and normal eaters’ hedonic responses gener- alizes further across time. We therefore included a third ISI of 1,500 ms in the modified AMP. Moreover, we wanted to inves- tigate whether the assessment of delayed hedonic responses has explanatory power beyond its link with dietary restraint. Specifically, we examined whether delayed hedonic respond- ing can be meaningfully linked to self-reported urges to eat palatable food. Method Participants were 48 students from Utrecht University (21 male, 27 female; mean age = 20.96 years, SD = 3.85 years), who received either course credit or monetary compensation 1866 Hofmann et al. (approximately $5). Two participants were identified as outli- ers in box plots and excluded from further analyses: One of them scored outside the normal range on the restraint measure (> 2.9 SDs from the mean), and the other reported not having
eaten for more than 18 hr (> 4.1 SDs from the mean).

All participants were preexposed to food cues through the
lexical decision task from Study 1. Afterward, their hedonic
responses to tempting food, matched positive control pic-
tures, and negative pictures were assessed with the modified
AMP. However, in Study 2, we added a third ISI of 1,500 ms
to the AMP, for a total of 135 trials, in order to trace hedonic
responding further over time. Participants then filled out the
restraint scale (M = 6.45; α = .75). They also completed the
Power of Food Scale (PFS; Lowe et al., 2009; α = .87; e.g.,
“If I see or smell a food I like, I get a powerful urge to have
some”), a recently proposed measure of sensitivity to tempt-
ing-food cues in the environment. Participants also com-
pleted the State Food Cravings Questionnaire (FCQ-S;
Cepeda-Benito, Gleaves, Williams, & Erath, 2000; α = .96;
e.g., “If I had tasty food right now, it would be hard to stop
eating”), which consists of five subscales—Intense Desire to
Eat (α = .95), Anticipation of Positive Reinforcement (α =
.90), Anticipation of Relief From Negative Feelings (α =
.88), Lack of Control Over Eating (α = .84), and Craving as
a Physiological State (α = .87). The PFS and the FCQ-S have
both been shown to predict problematic eating behavior in
field and laboratory settings (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000;
Lowe & Butryn, 2007; Vander Wal, Johnston, & Dhurandhar,
2007).

Results and discussion

A GLM analysis revealed a significant interaction among
stimulus type, ISI, and dietary restraint, F(2, 88) = 4.47, p =
.014, η

p
2 = .09. To unravel this interaction, we investigated the

effects of ISI and restraint on hedonic reactions separately for
food and positive control primes.

The analysis for food primes revealed a significant interac-
tion between ISI and restraint, F(2, 88) = 3.29, p = .042, η

p
2 =

.07; there were no significant main effects. As Figure 2 shows,
dieters and normal eaters did not differ in their hedonic
response at the short ISI of 100 ms, t(44) = −0.12, p = .908, η

p
2 =

.001. However, dieters’ hedonic responses were significantly
larger than those of normal eaters at the delayed ISIs of 1,000
ms, t(44) = 2.22, p = .032, η

p
2 = .10, and 1,500 ms, t(44) = 2.84,

p = .007, η
p
2 = .16. This dissociation of delayed responses after

preexposure to tempting food, which is analogous to the results
of Study 1, was due to a significant decline in hedonic respond-
ing for normal eaters from the 100-ms ISI to the 1,000-ms ISI,
t(44) = 3.16, p = .003, and from the 100-ms ISI to the 1,500-
ms ISI, t(44) = 2.63, p = .012. In contrast, and as in Study 1,
dieters maintained high levels of positive responding over
time—100-ms vs. 1,000-ms ISI: t(44) = 0.35, p = .73; 100-ms
vs. 1,500-ms ISI: t(44) = 0.43, p = .67. The analysis for posi-
tive control primes yielded neither significant main effects of
ISI and restraint (both Fs < 1) nor a significant interaction between ISI and restraint, F(2, 88) = 1.45, p = .24 (see Fig. 2), indicating again that the difference between dieters and nor- mal eaters was specific to food stimuli. 2.25 2.50 2.75 3.00 3.25 Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) No Preexposure Food Preexposure Food Stimuli Control Stimuli No Preexposure Food Preexposure H ed on ic R es po ns e (A M P ) Dieters (+1 SD) Normal Eaters (–1 SD) Fig. 1. Results from Study 1: hedonic responses to tempting food and matched positive control stimuli in the affect-misattribution procedure (AMP) as a function of food preexposure (present vs. absent), interstimulus interval (ISI; short vs. long), and dietary restraint (dieters vs. normal eaters: 1 SD above and 1 SD below the mean on the Concern for Dieting subscale, respectively). Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. Hedonic Responses to Tempting Food 1867 Next, we correlated hedonic responses to food in the AMP with self-reported power of food and the FCQ-S scales and sub- scales (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000) separately for the three dif- ferent ISIs. As shown in Table 1, correlations generally increased from the short to the longest ISI, and elevated positive responses to tempting food at the 1,500-ms ISI were substantially associ- ated with higher self-reported power of food, stronger desire to eat, stronger positive reinforcement from eating, more positive relief from eating, and less control over eating. To investigate whether late hedonic responding (ISI = 1,500 ms) can partially explain the relationship between dietary restraint and power of food in our sample, r = .445, p = .002, we conducted a mediation analysis, as recommended by Preacher and Hayes (2004). Results showed that the combined effect of restraint on delayed hedonic responses, β = 0.35, p = .016, and of hedonic responses on power-of-food scores, β = 0.51, p = .002, was significant, β = 0.16, p < .05, establishing mediation. A similar analysis involving FCQ-S (total) scores as the dependent variable also showed that the combined prod- uct of the two significant direct effects (β = 0.45 × β = 0.43) was reliable, β = 0.19, p < .05, indicating mediation. These analyses provide an intriguing link between dietary restraint and subjective experiences of urges and desires. They directly support our assumption that a failure to down-regulate hedonic 2.25 2.50 2.75 3.00 3.25 Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Longest ISI (1,500 ms) Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Longest ISI (1,500 ms) Food Stimuli Control Stimuli H e d o n ic R e sp o n se ( A M P ) Dieters (+1 SD) Normal Eaters (–1 SD) Fig. 2. Results from Study 2: hedonic responses to tempting-food stimuli and matched positive control stimuli in the affect-misattribution procedure (AMP) under preexposure as a function of interstimulus interval (ISI; 100 ms vs. 1,000 ms vs. 1,500 ms) and dietary restraint (dieters vs. normal eaters: 1 SD above and 1 SD below the mean on the Concern for Dieting subscale, respectively). Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. Table 1. Correlations Between Hedonic Responses to Tempting-Food Stimuli and Self-Report Measures in Study 2 Self-report measure Short ISI (100 ms) Long ISI (1,000 ms) Longest ISI (1,500 ms) Power of food .16 .26† .47* Food Cravings Questionnaire .17 .29* .43* Desire to eat .10 .16 .31* Positive reinforcement .27 .28† .43* Positive relief from eating .14 .34* .38* Lack of control .19 .31* .50* Craving as physiological state .06 .18 .26† Note: Hedonic responses to tempting-food stimuli were assessed with the affect-misattribution procedure (Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005). Self-reported cravings were assessed with the Power of Food Scale (Lowe et al., 2009) and the Food Cravings Questionnaire (Cepeda-Benito, Gleaves, Williams, & Erath, 2000). N = 46. ISI = interstimulus interval. †p < .10 (two-tailed). *p < .05 (two-tailed). 1868 Hofmann et al. affect over time may be one of the primary reasons underlying the propensity to overeat when tempted. General Discussion The present findings provide novel insights into when and why dieters may have difficulties resisting the allure of tempting food. Study 1 showed that without preexposure to food, dieters exhibited less positive hedonic responses to food than normal eaters did, an indication of successful devaluation of tempting food at both short and long ISIs. In contrast, when preexposed to palatable-food stimuli, dieters showed continued elevated hedonic responses, which differed markedly from the responses of normal eaters, who showed down-regulation over time. Study 2 replicated this finding and, by extending the ISI, dem- onstrated that the observed discrepancy remains stable for at least a certain amount of time. These results show that dieters are able to effectively regulate their hedonic responses to food as a result of having the goal to diet, but that preexposure to food triggers a hot state in which the down-regulation of hedonic responses is substantially impaired. These findings are consistent with the goal-conflict model of eating (Stroebe et al., 2008). According to this model, restrained eaters’ dieting goal successfully biases information processing, leading to a devaluation of goal-incompatible food. However, when a hot state is induced through repeated exposure to tempting food, the dieting goal is inhibited, and hedonic processing prevails. Unlike dieters, normal eaters are not chronically biased by a dieting goal and thus appear to be hedonically drawn toward tempting food under normal condi- tions. However, when preexposed to palatable-food stimuli, normal eaters (unlike dieters) showed reduced hedonic responding over time. This noteworthy effect may be part of a functional mechanism to disengage from stimuli that have already had a repeated influence so as to keep the organism open to the flexible pursuit of potential alternative goals (Jostmann & Koole, 2009). As our correlational and mediation analyses from Study 2 imply, dieters’ failure to down-regulate hedonic affect when tempted may be the key mechanism in the development of more elaborated food cravings and desires (Kavanagh et al., 2005) and may pave the way for unhealthy eating behavior. Our findings thus accord well with the recently proposed notion of hedonic hunger (Lowe & Butryn, 2007) by providing exper- imental evidence that it is precisely this prolonged responsive- ness to the pleasurable aspects of food that may put dieters at risk of unhealthy eating in today’s food-rich environments. The present findings also support the notion of automatic, implicit forms of self-control through the devaluation or inhibition of tempting stimuli (e.g., Fishbach et al., 2003; Fishbach & Shah, 2006; Hofmann, Deutsch, Lancaster, & Banaji, 2010). Results in the no-preexposure condition sug- gest that under default conditions, dieters can successfully counteract temptation through devaluation, even at a brief ISI. However, the observed pattern in the preexposure condition indicates that this regulatory mechanism may be substantially weakened through repeated confrontation with tempting cues, rendering dieters increasingly vulnerable to the hedonic aspects of food. This clearly supports the common observation that chronic dieters are often strong, but not invincible. Future research may help to further elucidate the boundaries under which otherwise beneficial mechanisms may be offset by spe- cific circumstances or stimulus constellations. Finally, our results may help to reconsolidate previous research into the hedonic-response hypothesis by incorporat- ing situational factors, such as preexposure and temporal dynamics, that might be overlooked in the prevailing focus on immediate automatic responses. Taking such a dynamic and context-dependent perspective, future self-regulation research from domains such as drug abuse, sexual risk taking, aggres- sion, and emotion regulation may likewise profit from a closer look at how immediate affect unfolds over time. Acknowledgments We thank Bertram Gawronski, Katie Lancaster, and Keith Payne for valuable comments. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Funding This research was conducted while the first author was a visiting fel- low at the Department of Social and Organization at Utrecht University. It was supported by a research exchange grant from the German Academic Exchange Service and by a grant from the German Science Foundation (HO 4175/3-1) to Wilhelm Hofmann, and by Grant 121510001 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research to Wolfgang Stroebe and Henk Aarts. Supplemental Material Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss.sagepub .com/content/by/supplemental-data Notes 1. Such a conditional view may help to explain some of the incon- sistencies reported earlier. For example, whereas the studies cited as reporting more negative evaluations of food by dieters did not involve a preexposure to food, Hoefling and Strack (2008) noted that, before completing the implicit measure in their study, “all subjects dealt with other food items (pictures) in another irrelevant task” (p. 684) involving the visual presentation of palatable-food stimuli (A. Höfling, personal communication, May 4, 2010). In a similar vein, Veenstra and de Jong (in press) noted that “an exogenous cueing task was …

Place your order
(550 words)

Approximate price: $22

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more
Open chat
1
You can contact our live agent via WhatsApp! Via + 1 929 473-0077

Feel free to ask questions, clarifications, or discounts available when placing an order.

Order your essay today and save 20% with the discount code GURUH